
- Dutch
- Frisian
- Afrikaans
- Dutch
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological processes
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Word stress
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Monomorphemic words
- Diachronic aspects
- Generalizations on stress placement
- Default penultimate stress
- Lexical stress
- The closed penult restriction
- Final closed syllables
- The diphthong restriction
- Superheavy syllables (SHS)
- The three-syllable window
- Segmental restrictions
- Phonetic correlates
- Stress shifts in loanwords
- Quantity-sensitivity
- Secondary stress
- Vowel reduction in unstressed syllables
- Stress in complex words
- Primary stress in simplex words
- Accent & intonation
- Clitics
- Spelling
- Morphology
- Word formation
- Compounding
- Nominal compounds
- Verbal compounds
- Adjectival compounds
- Affixoids
- Coordinative compounds
- Synthetic compounds
- Reduplicative compounds
- Phrase-based compounds
- Elative compounds
- Exocentric compounds
- Linking elements
- Separable complex verbs (SCVs)
- Gapping of complex words
- Particle verbs
- Copulative compounds
- Derivation
- Numerals
- Derivation: inputs and input restrictions
- The meaning of affixes
- Non-native morphology
- Cohering and non-cohering affixes
- Prefixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixation: person nouns
- Conversion
- Pseudo-participles
- Bound forms
- Nouns
- Nominal prefixes
- Nominal suffixes
- -aal and -eel
- -aar
- -aard
- -aat
- -air
- -aris
- -ast
- Diminutives
- -dom
- -een
- -ees
- -el (nominal)
- -elaar
- -enis
- -er (nominal)
- -erd
- -erik
- -es
- -eur
- -euse
- ge...te
- -heid
- -iaan, -aan
- -ief
- -iek
- -ier
- -ier (French)
- -ière
- -iet
- -igheid
- -ij and allomorphs
- -ijn
- -in
- -ing
- -isme
- -ist
- -iteit
- -ling
- -oir
- -oot
- -rice
- -schap
- -schap (de)
- -schap (het)
- -sel
- -st
- -ster
- -t
- -tal
- -te
- -voud
- Verbs
- Adjectives
- Adverbs
- Univerbation
- Neo-classical word formation
- Construction-dependent morphology
- Morphological productivity
- Compounding
- Inflection
- Inflection and derivation
- Allomorphy
- The interface between phonology and morphology
- Word formation
- Syntax
- Preface and acknowledgements
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of verb phrases I:Argument structure
- 3 Projection of verb phrases II:Verb frame alternations
- Introduction
- 3.1. Main types
- 3.2. Alternations involving the external argument
- 3.3. Alternations of noun phrases and PPs
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.3.1.1. Dative alternation with aan-phrases (recipients)
- 3.3.1.2. Dative alternation with naar-phrases (goals)
- 3.3.1.3. Dative alternation with van-phrases (sources)
- 3.3.1.4. Dative alternation with bij-phrases (possessors)
- 3.3.1.5. Dative alternation with voor-phrases (benefactives)
- 3.3.1.6. Conclusion
- 3.3.1.7. Bibliographical notes
- 3.3.2. Accusative/PP alternations
- 3.3.3. Nominative/PP alternations
- 3.3.1. Dative/PP alternations (dative shift)
- 3.4. Some apparent cases of verb frame alternation
- 3.5. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of verb phrases IIIa:Selection of clauses/verb phrases
- 5 Projection of verb phrases IIIb:Argument and complementive clauses
- Introduction
- 5.1. Finite argument clauses
- 5.2. Infinitival argument clauses
- 5.3. Complementive clauses
- 6 Projection of verb phrases IIIc:Complements of non-main verbs
- 7 Projection of verb phrases IIId:Verb clusters
- 8 Projection of verb phrases IV: Adverbial modification
- 9 Word order in the clause I:General introduction
- 10 Word order in the clause II:Position of the finite verb (verb-first/second)
- 11 Word order in the clause III:Clause-initial position (wh-movement)
- Introduction
- 11.1. The formation of V1- and V2-clauses
- 11.2. Clause-initial position remains (phonetically) empty
- 11.3. Clause-initial position is filled
- 12 Word order in the clause IV:Postverbal field (extraposition)
- 13 Word order in the clause V: Middle field (scrambling)
- 14 Main-clause external elements
- Nouns and Noun Phrases
- 1 Characterization and classification
- 2 Projection of noun phrases I: complementation
- Introduction
- 2.1. General observations
- 2.2. Prepositional and nominal complements
- 2.3. Clausal complements
- 2.4. Bibliographical notes
- 3 Projection of noun phrases II: modification
- Introduction
- 3.1. Restrictive and non-restrictive modifiers
- 3.2. Premodification
- 3.3. Postmodification
- 3.3.1. Adpositional phrases
- 3.3.2. Relative clauses
- 3.3.3. Infinitival clauses
- 3.3.4. A special case: clauses referring to a proposition
- 3.3.5. Adjectival phrases
- 3.3.6. Adverbial postmodification
- 3.4. Bibliographical notes
- 4 Projection of noun phrases III: binominal constructions
- Introduction
- 4.1. Binominal constructions without a preposition
- 4.2. Binominal constructions with a preposition
- 4.3. Bibliographical notes
- 5 Determiners: articles and pronouns
- Introduction
- 5.1. Articles
- 5.2. Pronouns
- 5.3. Bibliographical notes
- 6 Numerals and quantifiers
- 7 Pre-determiners
- Introduction
- 7.1. The universal quantifier al 'all' and its alternants
- 7.2. The pre-determiner heel 'all/whole'
- 7.3. A note on focus particles
- 7.4. Bibliographical notes
- 8 Syntactic uses of noun phrases
- Adjectives and Adjective Phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- 2 Projection of adjective phrases I: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adjective phrases II: Modification
- 4 Projection of adjective phrases III: Comparison
- 5 Attributive use of the adjective phrase
- 6 Predicative use of the adjective phrase
- 7 The partitive genitive construction
- 8 Adverbial use of the adjective phrase
- 9 Participles and infinitives: their adjectival use
- 10 Special constructions
- Adpositions and adpositional phrases
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Introduction
- 1.1. Characterization of the category adposition
- 1.2. A formal classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3. A semantic classification of adpositional phrases
- 1.3.1. Spatial adpositions
- 1.3.2. Temporal adpositions
- 1.3.3. Non-spatial/temporal prepositions
- 1.4. Borderline cases
- 1.5. Bibliographical notes
- 2 Projection of adpositional phrases: Complementation
- 3 Projection of adpositional phrases: Modification
- 4 Syntactic uses of the adpositional phrase
- 5 R-pronominalization and R-words
- 1 Characteristics and classification
- Phonology
- Frisian
- General
- Phonology
- Segment inventory
- Phonotactics
- Phonological Processes
- Assimilation
- Vowel nasalization
- Syllabic sonorants
- Final devoicing
- Fake geminates
- Vowel hiatus resolution
- Vowel reduction introduction
- Schwa deletion
- Schwa insertion
- /r/-deletion
- d-insertion
- {s/z}-insertion
- t-deletion
- Intrusive stop formation
- Breaking
- Vowel shortening
- h-deletion
- Replacement of the glide w
- Word stress
- Clitics
- Allomorphy
- Orthography of Frisian
- Morphology
- Inflection
- Word formation
- Derivation
- Prefixation
- Infixation
- Suffixation
- Nominal suffixes
- Verbal suffixes
- Adjectival suffixes
- Adverbial suffixes
- Numeral suffixes
- Interjectional suffixes
- Onomastic suffixes
- Conversion
- Derivation
- Syntax
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Unergative and unaccusative subjects
- Evidentiality
- To-infinitival clauses
- Predication and noun incorporation
- Ellipsis
- Imperativus-pro-Infinitivo
- Expression of irrealis
- Embedded Verb Second
- Agreement
- Negation
- Nouns & Noun Phrases
- Classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Partitive noun constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Nominalised quantifiers
- Kind partitives
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Bare nominal attributions
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers and (pre)determiners
- Interrogative pronouns
- R-pronouns
- Syntactic uses
- Adjective Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification and degree quantification
- Comparison by degree
- Comparative
- Superlative
- Equative
- Attribution
- Agreement
- Attributive adjectives vs. prenominal elements
- Complex adjectives
- Noun ellipsis
- Co-occurring adjectives
- Predication
- Partitive adjective constructions
- Adverbial use
- Participles and infinitives
- Adposition Phrases
- Characteristics and classification
- Complementation
- Modification
- Intransitive adpositions
- Predication
- Preposition stranding
- Verbs and Verb Phrases
- Afrikaans
- General
- Phonology
- Afrikaans phonology
- Segment inventory
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- The diphthongised long vowels /e/, /ø/ and /o/
- The unrounded mid-front vowel /ɛ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /ɑ/
- The unrounded low-central vowel /a/
- The rounded mid-high back vowel /ɔ/
- The rounded high back vowel /u/
- The rounded and unrounded high front vowels /i/ and /y/
- The unrounded and rounded central vowels /ə/ and /œ/
- The diphthongs /əi/, /œy/ and /œu/
- Overview of Afrikaans consonants
- The bilabial plosives /p/ and /b/
- The alveolar plosives /t/ and /d/
- The velar plosives /k/ and /g/
- The bilabial nasal /m/
- The alveolar nasal /n/
- The velar nasal /ŋ/
- The trill /r/
- The lateral liquid /l/
- The alveolar fricative /s/
- The velar fricative /x/
- The labiodental fricatives /f/ and /v/
- The approximants /ɦ/, /j/ and /ʋ/
- Overview of Afrikaans vowels
- Word stress
- The phonetic properties of stress
- Primary stress on monomorphemic words in Afrikaans
- Background to primary stress in monomorphemes in Afrikaans
- Overview of the Main Stress Rule of Afrikaans
- The short vowels of Afrikaans
- Long vowels in monomorphemes
- Primary stress on diphthongs in monomorphemes
- Exceptions
- Stress shifts in place names
- Stress shift towards word-final position
- Stress pattern of reduplications
- Phonological processes
- Vowel related processes
- Consonant related processes
- Homorganic glide insertion
- Phonology-morphology interface
- Phonotactics
- Morphology
- Syntax
- Afrikaans syntax
- Nouns and noun phrases
- Characteristics of the NP
- Classification of nouns
- Complementation of NPs
- Modification of NPs
- Binominal and partitive constructions
- Referential partitive constructions
- Partitive measure nouns
- Numeral partitive constructions
- Partitive question constructions
- Partitive constructions with nominalised quantifiers
- Partitive predication with prepositions
- Binominal name constructions
- Binominal genitive constructions
- Bare nominal attribution
- Articles and names
- Pronouns
- Quantifiers, determiners and predeterminers
- Syntactic uses of the noun phrase
- Adjectives and adjective phrases
- Characteristics and classification of the AP
- Complementation of APs
- Modification and Degree Quantification of APs
- Comparison by comparative, superlative and equative degree
- Attribution of APs
- Predication of APs
- The partitive adjective construction
- Adverbial use of APs
- Participles and infinitives as adjectives
- Verbs and verb phrases
- Characterisation and classification
- Argument structure
- Verb frame alternations
- Complements of non-main verbs
- Verb clusters
- Complement clauses
- Adverbial modification
- Word order in the clause: Introduction
- Word order in the clause: position of the finite Verb
- Word order in the clause: Clause-initial position
- Word order in the clause: Extraposition and right-dislocation in the postverbal field
- Word order in the middle field
- Emphatic constructions
- Adpositions and adposition phrases
In Dutch, we can distinguish between non-native, native, and nativized prefixes. Non-native prefixes only attach to non-native base words and non-native roots, while native and nativized prefixes also attach to native base words (Booij 1995). Prefixes usually syllabify independently, yet some non-native prefixes may be reinterpreted as forming one unit with their base word. In the majority of cases, prefixes do not attract main stress; yet they often carry secondary stress if they contain a full vowel.
Consider the following list of non-native prefixes, taken from Booij (1995):
apathisch | [a.ˈpa.tis] | apathic | |
absorbeer | /ɑb.sɔr.ber/ | [apsɔrˈber] | absorb |
adhesie | /ɑd.he.zi/ | [ɑtˈhezi] | adhesion |
antedateer | [ɑn.tə.da.ˈter] | antedate | |
apotheose | [a.po.te.ˈjo.zə] | apotheosis | |
concentreer | [kɔn.sɛn.ˈtrer] | concentrate | |
decomponeer | [de.kɔm.po.ˈner] | decompose | |
inclusief | [ɪn.kly.ˈsif] | inclusive | |
intervenieer | [ɪn.tər.ve.ni.ˈjer] | intervene | |
perforeer | [pɛr.fo.ˈrer] | perforate | |
prelude | [pre.ˈlyd] | prelude | |
postdateer | [pɔst.da.ˈter] | postdate | |
representeer | [re.pre.zɛn.ˈter] | represent | |
transponeer | [trɑns.po.ˈner] | transpose |
Non-native prefixes never carry primary word stress. Yet they can carry secondary stress if there is at least one intervening syllable between the first syllable of the prefix and the primary stress of the word. In that way, non-native prefixes behave like monomorphemic words; such words have have a predictable initial secondary stress – with the exception of cases in which the second syllable carries primary stress, as adjacent stresses within a prosodic word are disallowed and always have to be split by at least one unstressed syllable.
This predicts that monosyllabic prefixes should receive secondary stress if their base word does not have initial stress, while polysyllabic prefixes with a stressable first syllable should always receive initially stress on that syllable.
This is indeed the case. Consider first the prefix re-. For instance, in the word reductie [re.ˈdʏk.si] reduction, the prefix is followed by the main-stressed syllable of the word. Accordingly, the prefix remains unstressed and the vowel in the prefix re- can optionally be reduced to schwa ( [rə.ˈdʏk.si]), which is an indication that the main stress directly follows the prefix. Reduction, however, is impossible in representeer [(ˌ)re.pre.zɛn.ˈter] (I) represent, as the main stress is not adjacent to the prefix: secondary stress can be assigned to the initial syllable (since this would not result in a stress clash), and the syllable is not reducible. As the phonological makeup of these prefixed forms usually resembles that of monomorphemic words, and as their meaning is not transparent, it is often questionable whether they still classify as prefixes, or whether they rather form one unit with their base word.
The main distinction between non-native and nativized prefixes lies in their combinability with native base words. While non-native prefixes never combine with native base words, nativized prefixes can do so; some of them can even occur as independent words, as for instance anti-, co-, contra-, des-, ex-, inter-, meta-, non-, para-, pro-, semi-, sub-, super-, ultra- (see Booij 1995:120). Notably, these prefixes always receive primary stress when combined with nouns, and they never syllabify together with a noun; this indicates that, unlike non-native prefixes, they are independent prosodic words and form compounds with their base words. Consider the following examples:
anti-houding | [ˈɑn.ti.ˈhɑu.dɪŋ] | adversative attitude | |
copiloot | [ˈko.pi.ˈlot] | co-pilot | |
contragewicht | [ˈkɔn.tra.xə.ˈʋɪxt] | counterweight | |
desinteresse | [ˈdɛs.ɪn.tə.ˈrɛ.sə] | disinterest | |
ex-gelovige | [ˈɛks.xə.ˈlo.və.xə] | ex-believer | |
interfaculteit | [ˈɪn.tər.fa.kʏl.ˈtɛit] | interfaculty | |
metataal | [ˈme.ta.tal] | metalanguage | |
non-stop | [ˈnɔn.stɔp] | non-stop | |
semi-arts | [ˈsɛ.mi.ɑrts] | assistant doctor | |
subgroep | /ˈsʏb.xrup/ | [ˈsʏpxrup] | subgroup |
supermarkt | [ˈsy.pər.mɑrkt] | supermarket |
Note that in adjectival compounds, nativized prefixes do not attract primary compound stress – in such cases, primary compound stress falls on the adjective (in predicative position, while it shifts leftwards in attributive position; see Langeweg 1988):
intercontinentaal | [ˈɪn.tər.kɔn.ti.nɛn.ˈtal] | intercontinental |
paramedisch | [ˈpa.ra.ˈme.dis] | paramedical |
semidirect | [ˈsɛ.mi.di.ˈrɛkt] | semidirect |
ultramodern | [ˈʏl.tra.mo.ˈdɛrn] | very modern |
Largely based on Booij (1995), we divide native prefixes into three main classes with respect to their stress behavior. Some native prefixes attract main stress when they combine with a noun; such prefixes therefore behave like independent prosodic words, as they receive primary compound stress (at least when combined with nouns, which display the large majority of relevant words). On the other hand, there are native prefixes that never carry stress; these prefixes create verbs, and they also occur as particles or prepositions. There is also a small group of prefixes that, when used with base verbs, are situated in between the stress-attracting and the stress-avoiding ones; they sometimes attract stress: we refer to this group as intermediate.
a. | aarts-arts | |||||||
aartsbisschop | [ˈarts.bɪs.xɔp] | archbishop | ||||||
aartsvader | [ˈarts.va.dər] | patriarch |
b. | oer-ur | |||||||
oermens | [ˈur.mɛns] | primitive man | ||||||
oerwoud | [ˈur.ʋɑut] | jungle |
c. | on-ɔn | |||||||
onmens | [ˈɔn.mɛns] | brute | ||||||
onzin | [ˈɔn.zɪn] | nonsense |
d. | her-hɛr | |||||||
herbouw | [ˈhɛr.bɑu] | rebuilding | ||||||
herfinanciering | [ˈhɛr.fi.nɑn.ˌsi.rɪŋ] | refinancing |
e. | onder-ɔn.dər | |||||||
onderverhuur | [ˈɔn.dər.vər.hyr] | sublet | ||||||
onderbouw | [ˈɔn.dər.bɑu] | substructure, underpin |
f. | over-o.vər | |||||||
overgang | [ˈo.vər.xɑŋ] | transition, menopause | ||||||
overbesteding | [ˈo.vər.bə.ˌste.dɪŋ] | excess spending |
be- | /bə/ | bedijk | [bə.ˈdɛik] | to dike | |
er- | /ɛr/ | ervaar | [ɛr.ˈvar] | to experience | |
ge- | /xə/ | geloof | /xə.lov/ | [xəˈlof] | to believe |
ont- | /ɔnt/ | ontmoet | [ɔnt.ˈmut] | to meet | |
ver- | /vɛr/ | vertrouw | [vɛr.ˈtrɑu] | to trust |
The second group of stressless native prefixes contains prefixes that independently occur as prepositions. They all contain a full vowel: consequently, they meet the requirements of forming prosodic words on their own. As they do not receive primary compound stress, we can conclude that they do not behave like most nominal compounds (which show a strong-weak pattern). Their stress pattern rather resembles the weak-strong pattern found in a variety of adjectival compounds. However, while adjective compounds often show stress shifts in attributive position, such stress shifts do not occur for the prefixed verbal forms. The overview is taken from Booij (1995:122):
aan- | /an/ | aanvaard | /an.vard/ | [anˈvart] | to accept |
achter- | /ɑx.tər/ | achterhaal | [ɑx.tər.ˈhal] | to discover | |
door- | /dor/ | doorloop | [dor.ˈlop] | to pass | |
her- | /hɛr/ | herbouw | [hɛr.ˈbɑu] | to rebuild | |
mis- | /mɪs/ | misdraag | [mɪs.ˈdrax] | to misbehave | |
om- | /ɔm/ | omklem | [ɔm.ˈklɛm] | to grasp | |
onder- | /ɔn.dər/ | ondersteun | [ɔn.dər.ˈstøn] | to support | |
over- | /o.vər/ | overstroom | [o.vər.ˈstrom] | to overflow | |
vol- | /vɔl/ | voldoen | [vɔl.ˈdun] | to suffice | |
voor- | /vor/ | voorkom | [vor.ˈkɔm] | to prevent | |
weer- | /ʋer/ | weerspiegel | [ʋer.ˈspi.xəl] | to reflect |
Prefix | Base verb | Derived verb |
her- | halen [ˈha.lə(n)] fetch, get | (ik) herhaal [hɛr.ˈhal] (I) repeat |
formuleren [fɔr.my.ˈle.rə(n)] formulate | (ik) herformuleer [ˈhɛr.fɔr.my.leer] (I) reformulate, (I) rephrase | |
over- | komen [ˈko.mə(n)] come | overkoom [o.vər.ˈkom] |
belichten [bə.ˈlɪx.tə(n)] lighten | (ik) overbelicht [ˈo.vər.bə.lɪxt] (I) overexpose | |
onder- | schatten [ˈsxɑ.tə(n)] estimate | (ik) onderschat [ˈɔn.dər.sxɑt] (I) underestimate |
betalen [bə.ˈta.lə(n)] pay | (ik) onderbetaal [ˈɔn.dər.bə.tal] (I) underpay |
a. | Jan is onaardig. |
Jan is unkind. | |
ɔn.ˈar.dəx |
a.' | de onaardige Jan |
the unkind Jan | |
ˈɔn.ar.də.xə |
Alternatively, in predicative position, stress can be realized on on- “to emphasize the negative meaning”(Booij 1995:123). According to Booij, this is not possible in adjectives where on- is semantically opaque.
a. | Jan is onaardig. |
Jan is unkind. | |
ˈɔn.ar.dəx | |
[focus on negative meaning, positive counterpart aardig [ˈar.dəx] kind] |
a.' | *Jan is ondeugend. |
Jan is naughty. | |
*ˈɔn.dø.xənd | |
[(no positive counterpart)] |
The same applies to the prefixes aarts- and oer-(Booij 1995:123).
a. | De hond is aartslelijk. |
The dog is very ugly. | |
arts.ˈle.lək |
a.' | Een aartslelijke hond. |
A very ugly dog. | |
ˈarts.le.lə.kə |
b. | De man is oeroud. |
The man is very old. | |
ur.ˈɑud |
b.' | Een oeroude man. |
A very old man. | |
ˈur.ɑu.də |
Note that these forms display the same stress patterns like most adjectival compounds with respect to the stress alternation between predicative and attributive position (as opposed to nominal compounds which do not show stress shifts).
- Berg, Boudewijn van den1970Het woordaccent van afleidingen met het prefix on-De Nieuwe TaalgidsVan Haeringen-issue1-15
- Booij, Geert1995The phonology of DutchOxfordOxford University Press
- Booij, Geert1995The phonology of DutchOxfordOxford University Press
- Booij, Geert1995The phonology of DutchOxfordOxford University Press
- Booij, Geert1995The phonology of DutchOxfordOxford University Press
- Booij, Geert1995The phonology of DutchOxfordOxford University Press
- Booij, Geert1995The phonology of DutchOxfordOxford University Press
- Booij, Geert1995The phonology of DutchOxfordOxford University Press
- Booij, Geert1995The phonology of DutchOxfordOxford University Press
- Booij, Geert1995The phonology of DutchOxfordOxford University Press
- Booij, Geert1995The phonology of DutchOxfordOxford University Press
- Booij, Geert1995The phonology of DutchOxfordOxford University Press
- Langeweg, S. J1988The stress system of DutchUniversity of LeidenThesis
- Schultink, Henk1964De bouw van nieuwvormingen met her-Tijdschrift voor Nederlandse Taal- en Letterkunde80151-184
- Schultink, Henk1979Reacties op 'stress clash': de accentuering van samenstellende afleidingen, afleidingen van composita, en composita in het Nederlands: een eerste terreinverkenningSpektator8195-208
- Vries, Jan de1975Lexicale morfologie van het werkwoord in modern NederlandsLeidenLeiden University Press
